Ideomotion & Fascial Unwinding

Involuntary motion soft tissue techniques: Fascial Unwinding, Pandiculations and Muscle Repositioning

Fascial Unwinding

Minasny (2009) found fascial unwinding to involve two aspects:

  • Passively moving the patient in response to sensations of movement.

  • Inducing involuntary movement by using an initiation or induction technique.

Induction techniques initiates fascial unwinding that results in the patient responding with spontaneous expressions of movement in either a rhythmic or chaotic pattern.

The induction process is initiated by lifting and holding certain body parts to remove the influence of gravity as to overcome reactive proprioceptive postural tone. When the effects of gravity are removed, any strain patterns held in the tissues are more easily felt. The therapist follows any hint of movement without directing or forcing it. This involves the practitioner being largely passive but constantly aware of feedback from the patient's tissues (Minasny 2009).

Pandiculations

Schleip (2017) described another proprioceptive stimulating approach to produce involuntary movements called pandiculations. This was from the patient performing slow continuous resisted movement in a concentric and eccentric fashion whilst their soft tissues are being worked upon. This is usually repeated for sixty to ninety seconds followed by a brief isometric contraction of the antagonistic muscles.

Muscle Repositioning

Bertolucci (2010) described a technique called Muscle Repositioning that also worked on pandiculations. This author found involuntary movements to be produced as a result of internal shear forces among myofascial compartments. These shear forces are produced from the practitioner applying precise and sustained firm pressure at an oblique angle which produces a counter pressure generated from the inertia of the tissues.

The resultant involuntary movement happens in small increments (pandiculation), which become larger towards the end of the manoeuvre as body segments unite into a block. After the manoeuvre the patient often feels a burning sensation.

Somatic Experiencing 

Payne et al (2015) described the theory of Somatic Experiencing. The sympathetic nervous system may get “stuck” in a state of excess activation; this results in altered muscular activity disturbing the proprioceptive feedback that results in a failed reciprocal activation of the parasympathetic nervous system. This sympathetic-parasympathetic nervous system imbalance impacts on the neuroendocrine axis.

In Somatic Experiencing rebalancing the nervous system can be achieved by intense muscular effort and manual techniques producing involuntary spontaneous movements of the body such as gentle shaking and subtle postural changes. This is often accompanied by feelings of fear, sadness, or relief. It accounts for shaking and crying after an intense bout of sympathetic arousal and also, possibly, tonic immobility.

Therefore proprioceptive feedback is integral to allow the autonomic nervous system to reset to baseline. By drawing the patient's attention to the proprioceptive and kinesthetic (somatic) markers of this “release” process it enables a spontaneous rebalancing of the nervous system.

Viscoelastic changes in fascial unwinding (Blostein 2014)

In health the fascia network is straight, meaning that torsional forces in the fascia network are at a minimum. Thus the initial state is the configuration of lowest energy, in a liquid like state (‘sol’). Injury can introduce torsional forces that creates stiffness and introduces equal and opposite counter-torsion elsewhere in the fascia network.

However, adhesive forces can prevent a twist and counter-twist from meeting and cancelling out and hold the fascia network in a higher energy state. This ‘locked up’, high torsional stiffness, adhesive state moves the viscoelastic fascial system from a ‘sol’ like fluid state to a strong, solid like ‘gel’ state.

The aim of fascial unwinding is to overcome adhesions and bring together and cancel out twists and counter-twists. This cancelation of torsional forces aims to moves the fascia network from a locked up, higher energy, stiff ‘gel’ state back to a lower energy, fluid ‘sol’ state.

Proprioception 

Proprioception and involuntary motion

Ideomotion is the proposed mechanism for involuntary motion in fascial unwinding (Minasny 2009) and cranial osteopathy (Mason 2008); refer below 'Ideomotor theory' and 'Ideomotion and fascial unwninding'

Ideomotor actions are unconscious involuntary movements caused by prior expectations, suggestions, or preconceptions (Minasry 2008). Hence:

Ideation: thinking about or activating the mental representation of a perceived outcome and experiences --> motion: behaviour expressed through involuntary motor action. 

An example of an ideomotor activity is closing your eyes to go to sleep. An individual's representation (i.e. their ideation) of closing their eyes to go to sleep maybe the anticipation and mental representation of darkness, the closure of heavy eyes and unconsciousness. This leads to the non-deliberate, intuitive motor response (i.e. motion) of the individual closing their eyes (Wirth et al 2018).

Whilst all ideomotor responses look intuitive, some have to be learnt. For instance an ideomotor response would be an emotion e.g. positivity (ideation) triggering a motor response in the form of adopting a particular posture e.g. upright posture (motion). This would have been learnt when young through observation and role play. This ideomotor repsonse is bi-directional so when older and the association with posture and emotions are 'cemented in' to form a ideomotor reflex, activey changing a posture, in a particular situation can intern be therapeutic leading to a change of emotions.

The body's ability to preform these involuntary ideomotor actions are based, amongst other things, on proprioceptive feedback (Ondobaka & Bekkering 2012). Proprioception is not just centred around the awareness of the positioning of your body in space but the learning and emotions that accompany this (Liutsko et al 2016, Fuchs & Koch 2014 & Abraham et al 2020); refer below 'Proprioception and learning' and 'Proprioception and emotions'.

Therefore proprioceptive information, through learning and emotions, has a mental representation of the specific actions that had caused them. These learnt emotional mental representations form sensory anticipations that can trigger automatic ideomotor actions (Wirth et al 2016).

For example when performing a lumbar roll if a patient has a negative emotional representation of the technique causing tension and anticipation this can heighten proprioceptive feedback. This anxiety and proprioceptive feedback can trigger both voluntary tightness in the paraspinal muscles and a involuntary learnt ideomotor reflex response to such anxiety and vulnerability in the form of neck extension, facial muscle tension and hand and feet clenching. With the ideomotor reflex being bi-directional addressing these learnt ideomotor motor responses i.e. the neck extension, facial muscle tension and hand and feet clenching should reflexly both reduce anxiety and relax the paraspinal muscles for a more efficient technique. 

Much like with the emotional representation of sleep triggering the automatic motor response of closing the eyelids the learnt emotional and motor repsonses to proprioception are manifested through involuntary ideomotor movements (Ondobaka & Bekkering 2012). It is also why manual stimulation of proprioceptors has been associated with exciting the ideomotor reflex manifested with the involuntary motor movements in myofascial release (Minasny 2009) and cranial osteopathic techniques (Mason 2008).

What is proprioception

Proprioception means perception of ourselves, or more exactly, perception of the relative positions of the parts of our body (Liutsko et al 2016). 

Fascia is the richest sensory organ in the human body as a vast majority of sensory nerve endings in musculoskeletal tissues originate in it (e.g. perimysium and endomysium). Muscular-tendinous expansions insert into the fascia that transmit mechanical tensions to it. This in turn activates free nerve endings and other fascial receptors that contribute to accurate sensing of joint range of motion and positioning. Fascial stiffness has been linked with decreased proprioception and chronic sympathetic activation (Abraham et al 2020).

All this afferent information acts as “an anchor for self-awareness” (Liutsko et al 2016) as we perceive our self-awareness, feelings, mood, stress, energy and disposition from our physical bodies as a representative of all aspects of our physiological condition (Abraham 2020). 

Abraham (2020) identified sensory-proprioceptive information (or feedback) from fascia to include body contour and physical proportions; this information forms a mental representation of the body and its parts in space and in relation to each other (i.e. body schema). Disorders in body schema are reflected in connective tissue patterns.

Emotions and body states are closely interrelated, and modifications of one lead to changes in the other. Proprioception “encodes” these moods, feelings and attitudes so that have a bidirectional facilitation interference with movement. "happy movement <--> happy emotions".

This is why not only body sensations, but also body postures, gestures and expressions are inherent components of emotional experience that influence our evaluation of people, objects and situations, as well as memory recall (Fuchs & Koch 2014). 

This is exemplified by how the following movements and postures effect behaviour and emotions:

  • Approach movements have a bidirectional facilitation interference with positive moods, feelings and attitudes such as being excited, alert and determined (Liutsko et al 2016).

During an approach movement (e.g. arm flexion or receptive movement of the hands) an individual has a more positive evaluation of imagery and target objects (Fuchs & Koch 2014). 

  • Avoidance movements have a bidirectional facilitation interference with negative moods, feelings and attitudes such as being upset, guilty, and jittery (Liutsko et al 2016).

During an avoidance movement (e.g. arm extension or unreceptive movement of the hands) an individual has a more negative evaluation of imagery and target objects (Fuchs & Koch 2014).

  • When people stand or sit for 7 min in a “power position” (different forms of extension of the body), they perform better in subsequent job interviews (Fuchs & Koch 2014).

  • Shorter movements are associated with Inhibited people, while broader movements are associated with excited people (Liutsko et al 2016).

This association of emotion with proprioception and movement can also be compounded by other sensory inputs such as emotive language e.g. “love” and “hate” that are related with approach and avoidance gestures respectively (Liutsko et al 2016). Similar metaphors can also be used in visualisation or motor imagery (Abraham et al 2020).

So when individuals are able to adopt or produce emotion-specific postures, facial expressions or gestures they tend to experience the associated emotions, which effects their behaviour, preferences, judgement and attitudes toward objects or people.

Conversely, when an individual’s expressive movements are inhibited, this impairs their experience and processing of the associated emotions (Fuchs & Koch 2014). Mason (2008) found clinically this may manifest itself as isometric muscle contraction “holding yourself tight" or "holding tensions in”. 

There is also a bidirectional correlation with emotion and muscular tension and postural changes. Studies cited by Fuchs & Koch (2014) highlighted:

  • When slumped, individuals recall more negative life events; conversely more positive events are recalled when sitting upright.

  • Activation of the smiling muscles (by asking participants to hold a pen between their teeth) causes individuals to judge cartoons funnier than when smiling is inhibited by holding the pen between their lips.

The link between motion and movement is not only experienced by the individual but also by the observer. This can develop ‘kinaesthetic empathy’ where an observer perceives someone to move in a way that resonates with their own kinaesthetic representation of these movements. 

Therefore someones expressive behaviour affects the intensity of emotions experienced by not only the individual, but also the observer. This can be seen when experiencing emotions from someone's facial expressions or looking at professional dancers, musicians and sportsmen. This can also lead to congruent motor responses in the observer e.g. reciprocating to another’s facial expressions.

Developing kinaesthetic empathy from observation further reinforces our own personal value on proprioception for learning and developing emotional expression and emotional intelligence.

Proprioception and learning

When starting to learn a new skill, we rely more on abstract learning. This involves using concentrated attention and deliberate motor movements to observe and master the action we have been tasked with learning. We can only gain feedback on this deliberate movement, as to develop a perception or mental representation of it once we've completed and analysed the movement. An example of this would be when we first learn to write and we assess the size and smoothness of the lines once we've deliberately and consciously moved the pen.

With repeated practice we then learn on a proprioceptive level. This is where we start to look like we're operating on autopilot acquiring automatic or “embodied” knowledge (Liutsko et al 2016). Weimer et al (2001) attributed proprioceptive deficits as attributing to the "clumsiness" witnessed in Aspergers and is associated with these patients nonverbal learning.

Our perceptions or mental representations from proprioceptive feedback determine how we perform a task. An example of this would be observing how our writing unconsciously changes over the years once we can write fluently without conscious thought or how our emotions when for example writing an angry letter effect our proprioceptive feedback to determine non-conscious motor reponses in the muscles determining subtle changes in handwriting and pen pressure.

Embodied knowledge is the knowledge we obtain from all of our sensory, motor, and affective patterns. We process all this information to provide structure to our understanding so that we can engage with our world. This is different from an abstract intellectual grasping of concepts and their relations (Johnson 2015).

Proprioception is ideal for learning and processing this automatic or embodied knowledge (Liutsko et al 2016). This is because proprioception is key to bodily resonance, be it in the form of sensations, postures, expressive movements or movement tendencies (Fuchs & Koch 2014).

By using mimetic reproduction, from observing day-to-day movement, actions and expressions, proprioception is integral to learning cultural habits and the know-how associated with practical experience and professional skills (Liutsko et al 2016).

This process of learning forms and shapes development in a child when playing with toys. A child fuses movement and proprioception with emotion when they play with a favourite toy to create emboded imagery.

A child will identify with the toys qualities, movement and expression. They then emotionally and physically engage with the toy by moving it in an expressive manner transfering all this neural input to internally represent aspects of their own ego identity (Liutsko et al 2016).

It's not until five years old that a child can transfer behavior control from external to internal speech and inhibit their own responses (although they can inhibit their responses before this in response to external command). Therefore up until five a child has a blank cheque to fuse unhibited expression of movement with learning (Greenwald 1970).

This form of learning using all of our sensory, motor, and affective patterns is not only exclusively dependent upon proprioception. Many subconscious thoughts, emotions and perceptions play an influence in the process of observing and learning in this fashion. For example Fuchs & Koch (2014) identified studies showing how bodily felt warmth i.e. thermal heat from holding a hot drink, directly translates to impressions of emotional warmth. This can effect observations and perceptions during intuitive learning.

Proprioception and emotions

“The term “emotion” is derived from the Latin emovere, “to move out,” implying that inherent in emotions is a potential for movement, a directedness toward a certain goal (be it attractive or repulsive) and a tension between possible and actual movement” (Fuchs & Koch 2014). 

This is mirrored with the use of such language towards emotion where people describe being “moved” or “touched” or a “sinking feeling” or being “uplifted”.

Proprioception plays an important role in the construction of movements, formation of movement skills and in regulation of muscle tone. Proprioception also contributes to speech function or speech kinaesthesia and to general physical well-being and “sense of cheerfulness” (Liutsko et al 2016).

As the richest sensory organ in the body, fascial stiffness, from its contractile properties, fluid dynamics and myofascial relations has been associated with emotional stimuli. This could be as fascia’s efferent nerve endings account for more than 50% of its total nerve supply and are associated with a sympathetic response i.e. vasodilation. Sympathetic nerves are also located outside the vicinity of blood vessels questioning what their function maybe (Abraham et al 2020).

The correlation between proprioception and emotion can be reflected by its neurological links with:

  • Facial feedback: skeletal muscle afferent signals from facial expressions regulate emotional experience and behaviour.

  • Visceral feedback: visceral feedback from, for example, respiratory, heart function and the gut, are also correlated to emotional experience and behaviour.

This proprioceptive and interoceptive feedback from the body is integrated with more cognitive information in order to guide one's behaviour particularly with regards to every day decision-making (Fuchs & Koch 2014). 'Emodied cogntion' defines the bi-directional nature and how fundamentally potent the perception and representation of actions are to bodily and emotional experiences.

The relationship with emotions and bodily functions (including proprioception) where one can influence and manifest the other is illustrated in the quote: 

“We do not shiver because we are scared of the lion, but we shiver as this is what we feel as our fear” (James 1884, as cited in Fuchs & Koch 2014).

To extend this concept further other people can tickle you but you can't tickle yourself. Therefore how we process the perception of our emotions determines why what we feel as a tickle when someone else tickles us is different to what we feel as a tickle when we tickle ourselves. Consequently the tickle itself doesn't produce reflex bodily functions e.g. laugher and drawing away movements, but how we feel about, or process the tickle does.

How we feel about and neurologically process the information to determine how tickley something is is determined by how close the match is between the expected response and actual response. By tickling ourselves we remove the anticipation of the unknown causing the cerebellum to diminish activation in the somatosensory cortex (Simpson 2001). This reflexly determines the motor reaction to this processed afferent stimuli. Could this open the scope for mindfulness practice in addressing fear avoidance behaviour to alter the perception of proprioceptive and emotional feedback which will in turn modify their motor responses?

Therefore, feeling something and feeling oneself are inextricably bound together. This comes back to our the fundamental definition of proprioception by Liutsko et al (2016) of “the perception of ourselves”.

This emotion-somatic connection is bidirectional because just as an emotion (e.g. fear) will produce a somatic response (e.g. trembling) bodily (somatic) feelings produce an emotional response. For instance, being afraid is not possible without feeling oneself tremble, tense up, have palpitations, etc.

Other bodily systems are of course involved in this whole body systemic process e.g. smell, taste, auditory stimuli etc.

Any disturbances in life, stress, trauma and illnesses effects the proprioceptive state that both reflects in and is related to physical, emotional and cognitive functions (Liutsko et al 2016).

Liutsko et al (2016) identified examples of personality symptoms with disturbed proprioceptive function:

  • Autism: 80% of subjects with Asperger Syndrome displayed motor dyspraxia. Weimer et al (2001) identified proprioceptive deficits, rather than motor deficits, as explaining the incoordination observed in Asperger Syndrome

  • Clinically avoidant personality traits showed significantly poorer motor performance.

  • Down’s syndrome scores were significantly lower for both gross and fine motor skills, as well in running speed, balance, strength and visual motor control.

  • Bipolar disorder: demonstrates altered postural control.

  • Dysfunctions of both proprioceptive and sensory integration of proprioception and vision in personality disorders, aggressive behaviour and prison inmates.

Our bodies response to an emotional stimuli is its voice describing its “embodied appraisal” of a situation (Fuchs & Koch 2014); therefore proprioception has a critical role in listening to this appraisal and reorganising the subsequent recovery of these neuromotor systems (Liutsko et al 2016).

Involuntary motor reactions in response to involuntary motion soft tissue techniques

Examples of involuntary motor reactions are:

  • Isometric contraction of the cervical erector spinae during a suboccipital inhibition technique. This results in the practitioner’s hands being pressed into the table by the involuntary extension of the subject’s head and upper cervical spine. The cervical region may show a greater responsiveness than other body parts due to its richer proprioceptive innervation (Bertolucci & Kozasa 2010).

  • Eyelid flickering (Minasy 2009).

  • Horizontal eye movements (Minasy 2009).

  • Tremors (Minasy 2009).

  • Clonic and tonic appendicular movements (Minasy 2009).

  • Rising from a supine to a seated position (Minasy 2009).

Ideomotor theory

"The act comes first, the word proceeding from it as its concretized efflorescence" . Corporeal Words: Mikhail Bakhtin's Theology of Discourse Alexandar Mihailovic (1997). 

In predictive based models it is the intention of achieving a certain goal (e.g. grasping an object) that triggers the motor plan to achieve this intention. Based on this motor plan, one predicts how it should “feel” once this motor plan is achieved and the object has been grasped. The actual sensation whilst grasping the object is then compared with how one thought it would’ve felt in the predicted state and any deviations from this predicted state are bought to our attention; for example, you intend to reach for a cup without looking and this initiates a motor response. You’ve predict what a cup should feel like when your hand touches the object it doesn’t feel like a cup. This mismatch between what you predicted to feel and what you are actually feeling (‘prediction error’) gets bought to our attention so we can either initiate corrective movements, or, depending on the evidence, it should update how we represent what a cup should feel like.

In contrast, ideomotor reflexes and learning start with ‘motor babbling’. Motor babbling is characterised by purely random movements and behaviours that aim to practice or explore promoting learning centred around a self-rewarding curiosity (Haar et al 2020) (e.g. when babies explore their voice by saying “bababa”). Therefore, motor babbling is not orientated towards a predetermined goal or anticipated outcome, a baby doesn’t say “bababa” because it’s goal oriented towards refining its voice for public speaking, but it’s more of an exploration into the unknown ‘prod it and see what happens’. Over time associations are learnt between these motor actions and what is felt as a consequence of performing these actions which in effect tees up and primes the motor system e.g. learnt associations develop between the motor movement of saying ‘bababa’ and felt experiences such as noise, vibrations and received attention from other people.

Once these links have been established and any conflicting ideas have been removed (Massen & Prinz 2009) this process can be reversed; therefore, instead of motor actions instigating a felt consequence (‘I do something —> I feel something’), simply using our imagination, our mental representation, to recollect the consistently produced consequences of these motor actions activates motion in order to achieve these desired consequences or ‘goals’ (I feel something —> my autopilot elicits learnt motor responses’). An example of a central idea releasing, triggering and giving life to a teed up muscular system is whereby simply anticipating or predicting how it “feels” to grasp an object would activate the motor response; but because the strength of the induced action will depend on how strong the link is between what is being perceived and what are the perceivable effects of that action are (Massen & Prinz 2009) grasping a falling baby will drive a stronger motor movement than grasping a falling piece of paper.

This is why visualisation (motor imagery) by reframing sensory input and its motor responses has been shown to positively affect motor and cognitive performance and other behavioural outcomes e.g. anxiety, motivation, and confidence. It does this by eliciting brain activation similar to that during physical execution (movement, proprioception, pain, and body schema) to improve interoception and potentially even the physical structure of fascia as a sensory and motor organ (Abraham et al 2020).  

This bi-directional response-outcome (R-O) association (Sun et al 2020) can be summarised as:

  • Initially to achieve a desired response one learns:

Behaviour expressed through voluntary motor movements --> response --> perceived outcome. 

  • A learning phase ensues where a motor act triggering a bodily or environmental effect (Massen & Prinz 2008) becomes hard wired in the brain. This then results in the sequence of events being able to be performed in reverse:

Ideation: thinking about or activating the mental representation of a perceived outcome and experiences --> motion: behaviour expressed through involuntary motor action. 

Therefore in ideomotion focusing on and anticipating an outcome, e.g an emotion or environmental goal, will, enhance motor learning. In order for this process to occur two criteria must be met:

  • Clear goal. There has to be a clear idea or representation of what is being willed or intended. This goal has to resonate with the individual (Massen & Prinz 2008) as a compatible rewarding stimulus.

  • Blind positive association. All antagonistic impulses and thought should be removed (Mason 2008 & Massen & Prinz 2008).

Once these conditions are met the internal representations of what is intended has the power to evoke corresponding actions. This is because actions are represented through what their perceived to do and represent (Massen & Prinz 2008). Therefore ideomotor actions are intended to satisfy these perceptions and mental representations rather than be grounded in conscious abstract thinking.

Maquestiaux et al (2020) found ideomotion can be facilitated and encouraged when a task is ideomotor compatible. This is characterised by the individual recognising a high level of similarity between the stimulus and the associated response (e.g. seeing an arrow point to the left and then pressing a left key).

Conversely involuntary ideomotor activities can be voluntarily overruled and inhibited. This may occur when an expression of movement is culturally or socially unacceptable in which case it may become inhibited or suppressed. Clinically this may manifest itself as isometric muscle contraction (Mason 2008) “holding yourself tight" or "holding tensions in”. One can speculate if this can be transferred from practitioner to patient during a treatment i.e. if the practitioner is in a state of tension could this cause the patient to inhibit the free expression of their ideomotion?

This mechanism of ideomotor action has been used to explain various instances in which the environment triggers behaviours in an automatic fashion. For instance:

  • Mimicry (Sun et al 2020).

  • Behaviour from affordances e.g. pressing a button that looks like it should be pressed (Sun et al 2020).

  • Goal-directed behaviour and action priming (Sun et al 2020).

  • Body language, facial expression or body posture (Mason 2008).

  • Yawning (Mason 2008).

  • Postural correction (Mason 2008).

  • Closing your eyes to go to sleep (Wirth et al 2018).

Ideomotion and fascial unwinding

Ideomotor actions are unconscious involuntary movements that are performed by a person. It may be caused by prior expectations, suggestions, or preconceptions (Minasry 2008).

Ideomotor action has two important characteristics (Minasry 2008):

  • The patient is not aware of causing the movements, and therefore the movements are ascribed to an external force or power.

  • The movement feels unnatural, and thus the external forces perceived are usually regarded as being mystical or paranormal in nature.

Minasy (2009) ascribed ideomotion as the motion experienced by patient and practitioner during fascial unwinding. Mason (2008) also attributed it to the palpatory phenomenon described when performing osteopathy in the cranial field. It is proposed to work through three mechanisms (Minasy 2008):

  • Stimulation of fascial mechanorecpetors to produce reflex motor effects.

  • Suggestion or guiding of movement in a particular direction from the practitioner’s technique.

  • Promoting deep relaxation by ‘switching off’ tensions from the conscious mind. Stimulation of the fascial mechanoreceptors and suggestion of movement from the practitioner promotes ideomotion by working at a subconscious level. Whilst the motor movement from this subconscious processing is performed voluntarily by the patient (although it seems involuntary) and they are conscious of the movement the overriding tensions from the conscious mind that can inhibit this movement are ‘switched off’.

Cranial osteopathic techniques may possibly stimulate proprioceptors with direct intracranial effects. 

Schueler et al (2013 & 2014) found branches from the trigeminal nerve that innervate the dura mater and regulate blood flow intracranially also innervate extracranial soft tissues. These nerves, in the rat containing proprioceptive fibers (Schueler et al 2014), run a course originating intracranially to then traverse the cranium via the sutures and emissary canals to terminate extracranially. Extracranially these nerves innervate the connective tissue of the temperomandibular joint, periosteum and cervical muscles. Noseda et al (2019) proposed, not only can activation of extracranial muscle nociceptors cause headaches via their intracranial branches innervating the dura but also, in reverse, activation of intracranial dural nociceptors can give rise to extracranial muscle tenderness.

To initiate or facilitate fascia unwinding two conditions must be met (Minasy 2009):

  • The practitioner must posses sensitivity and fine palpation skills.

  • The patient must be able to relax and “let go” of their body.

Mason (2008) broadened the definition of ideomotion for the treatment of musculoskeletal disorders. This definition included the use of subconscious motor movements necessary to reach a state of comfort. This is achieved by removing the inhibition and suppression of instinctual motor patterns from pain or tension in order to facilitate and encourage ideomotor patterns to emerge (McCarthy et al 2007, Mason 2008). This can be achieved by regulating proprioceptive feedback.

References

Massen C and Prinz W (2008). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: an ideomotor approach to imitation (2008). 

Bertolucci L F (2010). Muscle Repositioning: Combining Subjective and Objective Feedbacks in the Teaching and Practice of a Reflex-Based Myofascial Release Technique

Sun D, Custers R, Marien H, and  AartsIdeomotor H (2020). Action: Evidence for Automaticity in Learning, but Not Execution 

Mason J (2008). The use of ideomotor therapy in the treatment of chronic neck pain: A single systems research design

Schleip R (2017). Fascia as a sensory organ: clinical applications 

Maquestiaux F, Lyphout-Spitz M, Ruthruff E, ArexisIdeomotor M (2020). Compatibility enables automatic response selection

McCarthy S, Rickards L, Lucas N (2007). Using the concept of ideomotor therapy in the treatment of a patient with chronic neck pain: A single system research design. 

Vogel D, Rudolf M, and Scherbaum S (2020). The impact of the verbal instruction and task characteristics on effect-based action control

Bertolucci LF, Kozasa EH (2010). Sustained Manual Loading of the Fascial System Can Evoke Tonic Reactions: Preliminary Results 

Minasny B (2009). Understanding the Process of Fascial Unwinding

Makin H, Hwang EJ , Hedrick NG, Komiyama T (2016). Circuit mechanisms of sensorimotor learning

Liutsko L, Tous JM, Maldonado JG (2016). How are you feeling? Proprioceptive keys of emotional intelligence

Fuchs T & Koch S (2014). Embodied affectivity: on moving and being moved

Payne P, Levine P & Crane-Godreau M (2015). Somatic experiencing: using interoception and proprioception as core elements of trauma therapy.

Johnson M (2015). Embodied understanding

Wirth R,  Pfister R, Brandes J, Kunde W (2018). Stroking me softly: Body-related effects in effect-based action control

Ondobaka S & Bekkering H (2012). Hierarchy of Idea-Guided Action and Perception-Guided Movement

Abraham A, Franklin E, Stecco C, Schleip R (2020). Integrating mental imagery and fascial tissue: A conceptualization for research into movement and cognition 

Simpson M (2001). Why can't you tickle yourself?

Noseda R, Melo-Carrillo A, Nir R, Strassman A, Burstein R (2019). Non-Trigeminal Nociceptive Innervation of the Posterior Dura: Implications to Occipital Headache

Schueler M, Messlinger K, Dux M, Neuhuber WL, De Col R. (2013). Extracranial projections of meningeal afferents and their impact on meningeal nociception and headache

Schueler M, Neuhuber W, Col R, Messlinger K (2014). Innervation of Rat and Human Dura Mater and Pericranial Tissues in the Parieto‐Temporal Region by Meningeal Afferents

GREENWALD A (1970). SENSORY FEEDBACK MECHANISMS IN PERFORMANCE CONTROL: WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO THE IDEO-MOTOR MECHANISM

WEIMER A, SCHATZ A,  LINCOLN A, BALLANTYNE A, TRAUNER D, (2001). ``Motor'' Impairment in Asperger Syndrome: Evidence for a Deficit in Proprioception 

Blostein D (2011). Fascial Unwinding Cancels Torsional Forces

Previous
Previous

Happiness & Positivity

Next
Next

Emotional Intelligence